May 7, 2014

Transcript
Lying to Ourselves

JAD: Hello. I'm Jad.

ROBERT: And I'm Robert.

JAD: And this is Radiolab. Today on our program, the topic is liars.

ROBERT: All kinds of liars. And now it's time for the liar we haven't yet mentioned, a liar which might actually be one very familiar to you, Jad.

JAD: What's that?

ROBERT: This is the self-deceiver.

JAD: Hey, what do you mean?

ROBERT: [laughs] Somebody—somebody who lies not to others but actually lies to oneself, if you get my drift.

JAD: Thanks, Krulwich. Thanks a lot. Anyhow, what does that even mean, to lie to oneself? How would you ...

ROBERT: It's tricky. Let me give you a classic example. Let's say that you are madly in love with somebody.

JAD: Who?

ROBERT: Just conjure up whoever you really, you know—I don't know who.

JAD: Okay.

ROBERT: So now you're in love with her, and strange things start to happen. You're at home. The phone rings. You pick it up.

JAD: Hello?

ROBERT: And the person on the other end of the line is breathing and then hangs up. Next, she's suddenly staying late at the office many nights a week. Didn't used to.

[WOMAN: Jad, honey. I've gotta work late tonight again. Don't wait up.]

ROBERT: Then your friends tell you that they see this woman ...

[FRIEND: So who's this guy?]

ROBERT: ... in the company of a man ...

[FRIEND: Does she have a brother, maybe?

ROBERT: ... repeatedly.

[FRIEND: Dude, come on!]

ROBERT: In short, all the signs are there. And yet despite the evidence, you, Jad, continue to believe—and I mean, you truly, truly believe that the woman is being faithful.

JAD: Well, maybe in this little scenario that you've created for me, I'm just stupid or clueless.

ROBERT: [laughs] Well, I'm not gonna take that away from you. I'm not. But in this case, though, for the sake of argument, let's say you're not clueless.

JAD: Okay.

ROBERT: Let's say you believe both these things in some different compartments in your head. You believe that she is faithful, and at the very same time, you know. You know what's really going on here.

JOANNA STAREK: What self-deception really is is that you have two contradictory beliefs, and you hold them at the same time, and you allow one of them into consciousness. And that you have a motivation for allowing one of them into consciousness.

ROBERT: That's Joanna Starek. She's a psychologist, and we're gonna hear more from her later.

JAD: All right, so how does that work then?

ROBERT: What?

JAD: What she just said, like, to have two contradictory thoughts in your brain at the same time, and yet you're only letting in one?

ROBERT: Well, there's an experiment on this subject, kind of an interesting one. And so ...

JAD: [sighs] Another experiment?

ROBERT: [laughs] Let me introduce you to the two guys who did it, okay?

HAROLD SACKEIM: I'm Harold Sackeim. I'm a professor in the departments of psychiatry and radiology at Columbia University.

RUBEN GUR: Okay. My name is Ruben Gur. I'm a neuropsychologist by training.

ROBERT: Harold Sackeim and Ruben Gur are friends. They met back in 1974.

RUBEN GUR: '73.

ROBERT: Ah, make that '73. One was a grad student—that would be Harold. One was a professor. That's ...

RUBEN GUR: Yeah.

ROBERT: ... that's Gur.

RUBEN GUR: And we started talking, and ...

HAROLD SACKEIM: To make a long story short, we did a couple of experiments. In one of them, we played clips of one's own voice and the voices of other people.

ROBERT: Here's the experiment: you, the subject, are sitting in a room, okay?

JAD: All right.

ROBERT: And we're gonna give you a big red button, and you can press it.

JAD: Okay, press the button.

[buzzer]

ROBERT: Not yet.

JAD: Oh, sorry.

ROBERT: And out of the speakers in this room, you're going to hear 10 different voices.

HAROLD SACKEIM: And everybody was saying the same thing. The words were the same.

[VOICES: Come here. Come here. Come here. Come here. Come here. Come here. Come here.]

ROBERT: And one of the voices in this group—one of the many—is you, Jad. You saying ...

[JAD: Come here.]

ROBERT: Right there. That was you. Now when you hear yourself saying "Come," press the button ...

HAROLD SACKEIM: Press the button: me or not me.

ROBERT: ... when you hear your own voice.

[VOICES: Come here. Come here. Come here.]

JAD: So one of these is mine?

ROBERT: Yeah.

JAD: All right.

[VOICES: Come here. Come here. Come here. Come here. Come here.]

JAD: Nope. No.

[VOICE: Come here.]

JAD: Yes.

[buzzer]

[VOICES: Come here. Come here.]

JAD: Not me. Not me. Not me.

[VOICES: Come here. Come here.]

JAD: Me.

[buzzer]

JAD: I think.

[VOICES: Come here. Come here. Come here. Come here.]

JAD: Not me.

ROBERT: Now if you listen very closely, it's gonna come in three, two—that's it!

JAD: Not me. Not me. Not me.

ROBERT: He missed it.

JAD: This is hard!

ROBERT: You're right! And the people in Harold's study, many of them didn't do too well, either.

JAD: So they had some trouble recognizing their own voice.

ROBERT: Mm-hmm.

JAD: All right. Bring it home, Robert. What's the point of this?

ROBERT: Here's what I didn't tell you: when they did this experiment in real life, the real subjects, in addition to having the little pusher button thing that we gave you ...

JAD: Yeah?

ROBERT: ... they also had diodes all over their body measuring ...

HAROLD SACKEIM: We recorded their physiology.

ROBERT: ... perspiration ...

RUBEN GUR: Skin sweating, heart rate ...

ROBERT: ... heartbeat, stuff like that.

RUBEN GUR: ... blood pressure.

ROBERT: And what they found is that when a person failed to recognize his or her voice, nevertheless, their bodies—the sweat, the heartbeat ...

HAROLD SACKEIM: Most often, the body is going "Ahh!"

ROBERT: Their bodies seemed to notice their voices, even though their conscious minds missed the voice. The body knew. The conscious mind didn't. Two thoughts in the same person.

JAD: Oh, come on. No. I mean, I'll give it to you. That's kind of interesting.

ROBERT: Thank you very much.

JAD: But that is not the same thing as lying.

ROBERT: Well, we're just starting here. We're just—this is—now at least grant me this: you can have two different experiences simultaneously.

JAD: Yes. Okay, I grant that you've just ...

ROBERT: Okay, so we're on our way. We're on our way. Now okay, step two: Harold and Ruben decide to leave the laboratory and go to a bar.

RUBEN GUR: Yeah, I believe it was Smokey Joe's.

ROBERT: Just to sort of talk things over.

RUBEN GUR: Kick back a bit. [laughs]

ROBERT: And to deal with your very question. Like, so let's really get to the core of what lying to yourself is about.

RUBEN GUR: Exactly.

ROBERT: So they're in the bar, and they're getting kind of drunk.

HAROLD SACKEIM: We were probably pretty drunk.

ROBERT: And Ruben proposes, we need to come up with some way to get test subjects to have one thought and instantly have a contradictory thought. Maybe we could do that with embarrassment. Maybe we could embarrass them into having two thoughts at the same time.

RUBEN GUR: And—yes, and at some point, I said, "Let's ask people questions."

ROBERT: Questions so ...

HAROLD SACKEIM: So threatening ...

ROBERT: ... so uncomfortable that you don't want to tell the truth about them.

JAD: Like what? What questions would those be?

ROBERT: Well ...

HAROLD SACKEIM: I mean, we had to get down and dirty.

ROBERT: They got drunker and drunker and drunker, and they came up with a whole bunch of them.

RUBEN GUR: Started writing them down.

HAROLD SACKEIM: It was fun.

RUBEN GUR: Right there in the bar on a napkin.

ROBERT: We were curious. So we took their questions off the napkin, so to speak, and we brought them out onto the street.

ELLEN: Can I ask you some questions while you're waiting?

MAN: Yeah, sure.

ROBERT: So here's one.

ELLEN: Have you ever doubted your sexual adequacy?

MAN: Ooh!

ELLEN: No.

ROBERT: And ...

MAN: Yeah.

ROBERT: ... another.

STAFFER: Have you ever enjoyed your bowel movements?

JAD: [laughs] Enjoyed my bowel movements?

STAFFER: Yep.

WOMAN: I think most normal people do.

MAN: No.

ROBERT: Here's another.

STAFFER: Have you ever thought of committing suicide in order to get back at somebody?

JAD: Yikes!

MAN: No.

ROBERT: And another.

STAFFER: Have you ever wanted to rape or be raped by somebody?

JAD: Come again?

WOMAN: No!

WOMAN: No.

MAN: Absolutely not.

WOMAN: No.

MAN: Oh, no.

MAN: Well, yeah.

JAD: Have I what?

ROBERT: Jad? [laughs]

JAD: What kind of question is that?

ROBERT: If you answered no to any of those questions, they would say that you're lying to yourself.

JAD: So they are assuming, then, that everybody enjoys their bowel movements secretly, everyone secretly has rape fantasies.

ROBERT: That's what they are assuming.

HAROLD SACKEIM: Yes, it was a supposition that these things are universal truths, but it was a supposition that seemed to work.

ROBERT: Because that night at the bar, Harold and Ruben stumbled across something: it turns out that how you answer those questions predicts some very surprising things about the kind of person you are, about the course of your whole life. First of all, remember that previous study we talked about with the voices?

JAD: Yeah.

ROBERT: It just so happens that the people who were very bad at the voice test, failed the voice test, they were the very same people who did very badly on the embarrassing questionnaire test. They didn't want to admit to stuff.

STAFFER: Have you ever wanted to rape or be raped by somebody?

MAN: No, not at all.

JAD: Huh.

ROBERT: However, when other scientists got ahold of Harold and Ruben's questionnaire—and they used it a lot in lots of situations ...

HAROLD SACKEIM: It's been given to thousands and thousands of people.

ROBERT: ... they dug deeper into the question of what do these people have trouble with truthiness, what happens to them in life?

JAD: Yeah. And?

ROBERT: And it turns out that they do a whole lot better in all kinds ...

JAD: Better?

ROBERT: Yes. Better, better, better, in all kinds of things.

JAD: Like what?

ROBERT: A whole lot of stuff.

JAD: Like?

ROBERT: Can we now say, by the way, that these people are liars?

JAD: I'm not quite ready to say that. But let's ...

ROBERT: All right.

JAD: Okay, fine. Let's just call them liars. And can you please tell me what the hell you're talking about? What sorts of things did they do better at?

ROBERT: Well, just to start, let me introduce you to someone.

JOANNA STAREK: Okay. My name is Joanna Starek, and I'm a psychologist.

ROBERT: Psychologist and athlete.

JOANNA STAREK: I was actually a swimmer. I was a competitive swimmer at Colgate University. And I think one of the questions that I was really interested in is: how can you have two people who have the same physiological capacity, and then one person over and over again would consistently win or outperform the other?

ROBERT: Joanna had heard about Harold and Ruben's questionnaire, so she and her research partner, Caroline Keating, decided to give the embarrassing question questionnaire to the swim team ...

JOANNA STAREK: Yes.

ROBERT: ... just to see what they'd find.

JOANNA STAREK: So we gave them that questionnaire at the beginning of the season, and then they trained, trying to qualify for the Eastern Athletic Conference championship.

ROBERT: That's the big race at the end of the year.

JOANNA STAREK: And it's a very objective measure. You either swim fast enough during the season to qualify or you don't.

ROBERT: And when, at the end of the season, Joanna and her research partner Caroline looked at which swimmers did the best, which ones qualified ...

JOANNA STAREK: We did find a bizarre relationship.

ROBERT: ... the swimmers who said—the ones who were the liars, who said no to all these questions ...

JOANNA STAREK: Do you enjoy your bowel movements?

WOMAN: No.

JOANNA STAREK: Have you ever thought about killing yourself?

WOMAN: No.

JOANNA STAREK: Have you ever thought about raping someone?

MAN: No.

ROBERT: ... consistently ...

JOANNA STAREK: They were the winners.

ROBERT: The fastest and most successful swimmers were the ones who, on the questionnaire, according to Harold and Ruben, lied to themselves.

JOANNA STAREK: Yes. I do think a little bit of deception is not necessarily a bad thing.

ROBERT: It might even be a crucial thing. Just for example, I want you to listen to these Olympic track athletes. We got these interview clips from the sound artist Ben Rubin. And listen to how these athletes describe the process of getting ready to race.

[MALE ATHLETE #1: We believe we're invincible because if we go in there with any other thought, there's no chance of us accomplishing our goal.]

[FEMALE ATHLETE #1: Well, of course, I always win in my thoughts. [laughs]]

[MALE ATHLETE #2: I have the ability to catch this person. It's gonna happen.]

[FEMALE ATHLETE #2: Take your head off. Leave your head at home. Leave your brain at home today.]

[FEMALE ATHLETE #3: When I step on the runway, I just relax myself. You are the best. And I go.]

ROBERT: And more than sports, denying certain facts about the real world around you, according to any number of new studies, produces people who, turns out, are better at business and better at working with teams. And now here's the real kicker: they turn out to be happier people.

HAROLD SACKEIM: We—the questionnaire served a couple of purposes. One of the things that it taught us is that people who were happiest were the ones who were lying to themselves more.

JOANNA STAREK: The people who are the most realistic, that actually see the world exactly as it is, tend to be slightly more depressed than others.

ROBERT: Time and time again, researchers have found that depressed people lie less.

HAROLD SACKEIM: They see all the pain in the world, how horrible people are with each other, and they tell you everything about themselves: what their weaknesses are, what terrible things they've done to other people. And the problem is they're right. And so maybe it's the way we help people is to help them be wrong.

ROBERT: It might just be that hiding ideas that we know to be true, hiding those ideas from ourselves, is what we need to get by.

HAROLD SACKEIM: We're so vulnerable to being hurt that we're given the capacity to distort as a gift.

JAD: Well, that's it for us. If you want any more information on anything you heard this hour, check our website, Radiolab.org.

ROBERT: I'm Robert Krulwich.

JAD: I'm Jad Abumrad.

ROBERT: And this is Radiolab.

JAD: Thanks for listening.

[ANSWERING MACHINE: You have two new messages.]

[JUDE HOFFNER: Radiolab is produced by Jad Abumrad with Lulu Miller, Rob Christiansen, Ellen Horne, Justin Paul and Soren Wheeler. Production support by Amber Seely, Lasca Kebbell, Jed Teres, Sara Pellegrini, Arielle Lasky, Heather Radke, Michael O'Ryan McManus and Sally Herships. Special thanks to me, Jude Hoffner, Jane Dumestre ...]

[RUBEN GUR: ... and Scott Robinson. Radiolab is funded in part by the Alfred P. Sloan Foundation, the Corporation for Public Broadcasting and the National Science Foundation. Radiolab is produced by WNYC, New York Public Radio, and distributed by NPR, National Public Radio. Bye.]

 

-30-

 

Copyright © 2023 New York Public Radio. All rights reserved. Visit our website terms of use at www.wnyc.org for further information.

 

New York Public Radio transcripts are created on a rush deadline, often by contractors. This text may not be in its final form and may be updated or revised in the future. Accuracy and availability may vary. The authoritative record of programming is the audio record.

THE LAB sticker

Unlock member-only exclusives and support the show

Exclusive Podcast Extras
Entire Podcast Archive
Listen Ad-Free
Behind-the-Scenes Content
Video Extras
Original Music & Playlists